Moroni, C. & Bayard, S. (2009). Processus d’inhibition : Quelle est leur évolution après 50 ans ? Psychologie et Neuropsychiatrie du vieillissement, 2, 121-9.
Bayard, S., Erkes, J., Moroni,C. & les Membres du Collège des Psychologues spécialisés en Neuropsychologie du Languedoc-Roussillon (2011). Victoria Stroop Test: Normative data in a sample group of older people and the study of their clinical applications in the assessment of inhibition in Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neuropsychology, 26(7), 653-61.
Mahieux, F., Fabre, C., Galbrun, E., Dubrulle, A. & Moroni, C. et le groupe de réflexion sur les praxies du CMRR Ile de France-Sud (2009). Validation d’une batterie brève d’évaluation des praxies gestuelles pour Consultation Mémoire. Evaluation chez 419 témoins, 127 MCI et 320 patients souffrant d’une démence. Revue Neurologique, 165, 560-67.
Delbeuck, X. Boutantin, J., Leroy, A., Pollet, M., Lebert, F., Pasquier, F., & Moroni, C. (2011). Utilité clinique du test des Faux Pas pour le diagnostic de démence fronto-temporale. Intervention dans le cadre des Journées d’Hiver de la Société de Neuropsychologie de Langue Française, Paris, 2 décembre 2011 [communication écrite].
Delbeuck, X., Debachy, B., Pasquier, F. & Moroni, C. (2013). Action and noun fluence testing to distinguish between Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. DOI:10.1080/13803395.2013.763907
Bayard, S., Gély-Nargeot M-C, Raffard, S., Guerdoux-Ninot, E., Kamara, E., Gros-Balthazard, F., Jacus J-P & Moroni C., The Collège des Psychologues Cliniciens spécialisés en Neuropsychologie du Languedoc Roussillon (CPCN-Languedoc Roussillon) (accepted).French version of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test, Part I : Normative date and guidelines for error scoring. Archives of Neuropsychology.
Bayard,S., Moroni, C., Gély-Nargeot, M-C, Rossignol-Arifi,A., Kamara, E. & RaffardS. (Accepted). French version of the Hayling Sentence Clompletion Test, Part II : Clinical utility in schizophrenia and Parkinson’s disease. Archives of Neuropsychology
2020
|
1. | Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Helene; Daveluy, Walter; Torre, Fabien; Moroni, Christine Machine learning modelling of neuropsychological performance could determine vocational training outcome after a brain injury: case report Article de journal Dans: Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 2020. Liens | BibTeX @article{Manoli2020,
title = {Machine learning modelling of neuropsychological performance could determine vocational training outcome after a brain injury: case report},
author = {Romina Manoli and Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Helene Delecroix and Walter Daveluy and Fabien Torre and Christine Moroni},
doi = {10.1016/j.rehab.2020.01.009},
year = {2020},
date = {2020-04-11},
journal = {Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2. | Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Helene; Daveluy, Walter; Moroni, Christine Is Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI-20) adequate to measure brain injury related fatigue? Article de journal Dans: Disability and Health Journal, 2020. Liens | BibTeX @article{Manoli2020b,
title = {Is Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI-20) adequate to measure brain injury related fatigue?},
author = {Romina Manoli and Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Helene Delecroix and Walter Daveluy and Christine Moroni
},
doi = {10.1016/j.dhjo.2020.100913},
year = {2020},
date = {2020-03-26},
journal = {Disability and Health Journal},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
3. | Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Helene; Daveluy, Walter; Torre, Fabien; Moroni, Christine The relationship between cognition and vocational training outcome in patients with acquired brain injury: Contribution of Machine Learning Article de journal Dans: Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 2020. Liens | BibTeX @article{Manoli2020c,
title = {The relationship between cognition and vocational training outcome in patients with acquired brain injury: Contribution of Machine Learning},
author = {Romina Manoli and Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Helene Delecroix and Walter Daveluy and Fabien Torre and Christine Moroni},
doi = {10.1080/23279095.2020.1734809},
year = {2020},
date = {2020-03-06},
journal = {Applied Neuropsychology: Adult},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2019
|
4. | Manoli, Romina; Delecroix, Hélène; Daveluy, Walter; Moroni, Christine Impact of cognitive and behavioural functioning on vocational outcome following traumatic brain injury: a systematic review Article de journal Dans: Disability and Rehabilitation, 2019. Liens | BibTeX @article{Manoli2019,
title = {Impact of cognitive and behavioural functioning on vocational outcome following traumatic brain injury: a systematic review},
author = {Romina Manoli and Hélène Delecroix and Walter Daveluy and Christine Moroni},
editor = {Taylor & Francis},
doi = {10.1080/09638288.2019.1706105},
year = {2019},
date = {2019-12-22},
journal = {Disability and Rehabilitation},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2018
|
5. | Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Hélène; Daveluy, Walter; Moroni, Christine Intérêt de l’évaluation des capacités de consolidation à long terme des patients avec un traumatisme crânien grave 5500 - 5599 Conférence Journée d'Hiver de la Société de Neuropsychologie de Langue Française (Prix de la meilleure communication orale), 2018. BibTeX @conference{Manoli2018f,
title = {Intérêt de l’évaluation des capacités de consolidation à long terme des patients avec un traumatisme crânien grave},
author = {Romina Manoli and Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Hélène Delecroix and Walter Daveluy and Christine Moroni},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-12-07},
booktitle = {Journée d'Hiver de la Société de Neuropsychologie de Langue Française (Prix de la meilleure communication orale)},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
6. | Manoli Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix Hélène; Daveluy Walter; Moroni Christine Why evaluate one-week-delayed verbal recall in patients with severe traumatic brain injury? Article de journal Dans: Brain Injury, 2018. Liens | BibTeX @article{Manoli2018e,
title = {Why evaluate one-week-delayed verbal recall in patients with severe traumatic brain injury?},
author = {Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Hélène; Daveluy, Walter; Moroni, Christine},
doi = {10.1080/02699052.2018.1539245},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-10-25},
journal = {Brain Injury},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
7. | Manoli Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix Helene; Daveluy Walter; Torre Fabien; Moroni Christine How can we anticipate failure in resuming vocational training following brain injury? A retrospective study 5500 - 5599 Conférence 12th International Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine World Congress, Paris (France), e-poster , 2018. BibTeX @conference{Manoli2018d,
title = {How can we anticipate failure in resuming vocational training following brain injury? A retrospective study},
author = {Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Helene; Daveluy, Walter; Torre, Fabien; Moroni, Christine
},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-07-08},
booktitle = {12th International Society of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine World Congress, Paris (France)},
volume = {e-poster},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
8. | Manoli Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix Hélène; Daveluy Walter; Torre Fabien; Moroni Christine What Cognitive Factors Would Predict The Return To Vocational Training In Adults With Brain Injury? 5500 - 5599 Conférence 29th International Congress of Applied Psychology, Montréal (Canada), Oral communication , 2018. BibTeX @conference{Manoli2018b,
title = {What Cognitive Factors Would Predict The Return To Vocational Training In Adults With Brain Injury?},
author = {Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Hélène; Daveluy, Walter; Torre, Fabien; Moroni, Christine},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-06-26},
booktitle = {29th International Congress of Applied Psychology, Montréal (Canada)},
volume = {Oral communication},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
9. | Manoli Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Daveluy Walter; Delecroix Hélène; Noulhiane Marion; Moroni Christine New measures for long-term verbal memory consolidation assessment 5500 - 5599 Conférence 29th International Congress of Applied Psychology, Montréal (Canada), Oral communication , 2018. BibTeX @conference{Manoli2018c,
title = {New measures for long-term verbal memory consolidation assessment},
author = {Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Daveluy, Walter; Delecroix, Hélène; Noulhiane, Marion; Moroni, Christine},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-06-26},
booktitle = {29th International Congress of Applied Psychology, Montréal (Canada)},
volume = {Oral communication},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
10. | Manoli Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix Hélène; Daveluy Walter; Torre Fabien; Moroni Christine Predicting vocational outcome following brain injury: towards a better professional guidance 5500 - 5599 Conférence The 42nd Brain Injury Rehabilitation Conference, Williamsburg (Virginia, USA), Oral communication , 2018. BibTeX @conference{Manoli2018,
title = {Predicting vocational outcome following brain injury: towards a better professional guidance},
author = {Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Hélène; Daveluy, Walter; Torre, Fabien; Moroni, Christine},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-05-04},
booktitle = {The 42nd Brain Injury Rehabilitation Conference, Williamsburg (Virginia, USA)},
volume = {Oral communication},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
11. | Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Mecheri, Halima; Delecroix, Hélène; Noulhiane, Marion; Moroni, Christine Extension to one week of verbal memory consolidation assessment Article de journal Dans: Neuropsychology, 32 (3), p. 366-373, 2018. Liens | BibTeX @article{Manoli2017,
title = {Extension to one week of verbal memory consolidation assessment},
author = {Romina Manoli and Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Halima Mecheri and Hélène Delecroix and Marion Noulhiane and Christine Moroni},
doi = {10.1037/neu0000403},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-03-01},
journal = {Neuropsychology},
volume = {32},
number = {3},
pages = {366-373},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2017
|
12. | Manoli, Romina; Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Delecroix, Hélène; Daveluy, Walter; Moroni, Christine Long-term forgetting and memory impairment in patients with traumatic brain injury Workshop Cognitive neuroscience of memory: The Recollection, familiarity and novelty detection, University of Liege (Belgium), Poster , 2017. BibTeX @workshop{Manoli2017b,
title = {Long-term forgetting and memory impairment in patients with traumatic brain injury},
author = {Romina Manoli and Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Hélène Delecroix and Walter Daveluy and Christine Moroni},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-03-24},
booktitle = {Cognitive neuroscience of memory: The Recollection, familiarity and novelty detection, University of Liege (Belgium)},
volume = {Poster},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {workshop}
}
|
13. | Bayard, Sophie; Moroni, Christine; Gély-Nargeot, Marie-Christine; Arifi, Alexia Rossignol-; Kamara, Emmanuelle; Raffard, Stéphane French Version of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test, Part II: Clinical Utility in Schizophrenia and Parkinson's Disease Article de journal Dans: Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, p. 1–6, 2017, ISSN: 0887-6177, 1873-5843. Liens | BibTeX @article{bayard_french_2017-1,
title = {French Version of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test, Part II: Clinical Utility in Schizophrenia and Parkinson's Disease},
author = {Sophie Bayard and Christine Moroni and Marie-Christine Gély-Nargeot and Alexia Rossignol- Arifi and Emmanuelle Kamara and Stéphane Raffard},
url = {https://academic.oup.com/acn/acn/article/2975331/French},
doi = {10.1093/arclin/acx011},
issn = {0887-6177, 1873-5843},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-02-01},
urldate = {2017-08-28},
journal = {Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology},
pages = {1--6},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
14. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine A rehabilitation program based on music practice for patients with unilateral spatial neglect: a single-case study Article de journal Dans: Neurocase, 23 (1), p. 12–21, 2017, ISSN: 1355-4794. Liens | BibTeX @article{guilbert_rehabilitation_2017,
title = {A rehabilitation program based on music practice for patients with unilateral spatial neglect: a single-case study},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
doi = {10.1080/13554794.2016.1265652},
issn = {1355-4794},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Neurocase},
volume = {23},
number = {1},
pages = {12--21},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
15. | Bayard, Sophie; Gély-Nargeot, Marie-Christine; Raffard, Stéphane; Guerdoux-Ninot, Estelle; Kamara, Emmanuelle; Gros-Balthazard, Florent; Jacus, Jean-Pierre; Moroni, Christine French Version of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test, Part I: Normative Data and Guidelines for Error Scoring Article de journal Dans: Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, p. 1–7, 2017, ISSN: 0887-6177, 1873-5843. Liens | BibTeX @article{bayard_french_2017,
title = {French Version of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test, Part I: Normative Data and Guidelines for Error Scoring},
author = {Sophie Bayard and Marie-Christine Gély-Nargeot and Stéphane Raffard and Estelle Guerdoux-Ninot and Emmanuelle Kamara and Florent Gros-Balthazard and Jean-Pierre Jacus and Christine Moroni},
url = {https://academic.oup.com/acn/acn/article/2972111/French},
doi = {10.1093/arclin/acx010},
issn = {0887-6177, 1873-5843},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
urldate = {2017-08-28},
journal = {Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology},
pages = {1--7},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2016
|
16. | Moroni, Christine; Narme, Pauline 20 cas cliniques en Neuropsychologie. Livre 2016, (Enfant, adulte, personne âgée). Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @book{Moroni2016,
title = {20 cas cliniques en Neuropsychologie. },
author = {Christine Moroni and Pauline Narme },
editor = {Dunod},
url = {https://www.dunod.com/sciences-humaines-et-sociales/20-cas-cliniques-en-neuropsychologie-enfant-adulte-personne-agee},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-05-11},
abstract = {L’évaluation neuropsychologique repose sur un ensemble de connaissances théoriques couplé à une méthodologie spécifique. Cet ouvrage rappelle les principes méthodologiques fondamentaux de la démarche du bilan neuropsychologique à visée...},
note = {Enfant, adulte, personne âgée},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {book}
}
L’évaluation neuropsychologique repose sur un ensemble de connaissances théoriques couplé à une méthodologie spécifique. Cet ouvrage rappelle les principes méthodologiques fondamentaux de la démarche du bilan neuropsychologique à visée... |
17. | Guilbert, Alma; Guillaume, Cécile Evaluation d'une plainte mnésique non authentifiée par un bilan standard incollection Dans: Moroni, Christine ; Narme, Pauline (Ed.): 20 cas cliniques en Neuropsychologie , 2016. BibTeX @incollection{guilbert_evaluation_2016,
title = {Evaluation d'une plainte mnésique non authentifiée par un bilan standard},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Cécile Guillaume},
editor = {Moroni, Christine and Narme, Pauline},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-05-01},
volume = {20 cas cliniques en Neuropsychologie},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {incollection}
}
|
18. | Moroni, Christine Evaluation neuropsychologique en cas de troubles sensoriels incollection Dans: L'évaluation neuropsychologique de la norme à l'exception / sous la direction d'Hélène Amieva, Catherine Belin et Didier Maillet, p. 125–136, De Boeck- Supérieur, 2016, ISBN: 978-2-35327-352-2. Liens | BibTeX @incollection{moroni_evaluation_2016,
title = {Evaluation neuropsychologique en cas de troubles sensoriels},
author = {Christine Moroni},
url = {http://www.deboecksuperieur.com/ouvrage/9782353273522-levaluation-neuropsychologique},
isbn = {978-2-35327-352-2},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-05-01},
booktitle = {L'évaluation neuropsychologique de la norme à l'exception / sous la direction d'Hélène Amieva, Catherine Belin et Didier Maillet},
pages = {125--136},
publisher = {De Boeck- Supérieur},
edition = {1re édition},
series = {Neuropsychologie},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {incollection}
}
|
19. | Moroni, Christine Transfert inter-hémisphérique et activité motrice involontaire (Alien Hand). incollection Dans: 20 cas cliniques en neuropsychologie / sous la direction de Christine Moroni, Pauline Narme, p. 119–133, Dunod, 2016, ISBN: 978-2-10-074023-9. BibTeX @incollection{moroni_transfert_2016,
title = {Transfert inter-hémisphérique et activité motrice involontaire (Alien Hand).},
author = {Christine Moroni},
isbn = {978-2-10-074023-9},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-05-01},
booktitle = {20 cas cliniques en neuropsychologie / sous la direction de Christine Moroni, Pauline Narme},
pages = {119--133},
publisher = {Dunod},
series = {Psycho sup},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {incollection}
}
|
20. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine Musical practice as a way to rehabilitate unilateral spatial neglect: A single case study Inproceedings Dans: 2016. BibTeX @inproceedings{Guilbert2016,
title = {Musical practice as a way to rehabilitate unilateral spatial neglect: A single case study},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-03-01},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {inproceedings}
}
|
21. | Durand, Thomas; Léger, Isabelle; Bompaire, Flavie; Boone, Mathieu; Moroni, Christine; Delgadillo, Daniel; Dornier, Stéphanie; Psimaras, Dimitri; Noël, Georges; Taillia, Hervé; Ricard, Damien Tumeurs cérébrales : neurotoxicité des traitements et évaluations cognitives Article de journal Dans: Revue de neuropsychologie, 8 (3), p. 192–200, 2016, ISSN: 2101-6739. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{durand_tumeurs_2016,
title = {Tumeurs cérébrales : neurotoxicité des traitements et évaluations cognitives},
author = {Thomas Durand and Isabelle Léger and Flavie Bompaire and Mathieu Boone and Christine Moroni and Daniel Delgadillo and Stéphanie Dornier and Dimitri Psimaras and Georges Noël and Hervé Taillia and Damien Ricard},
url = {http://www.cairn.info/resume.php?ID_ARTICLE=RNE_083_0192},
doi = {doi:10.1684/nrp.2016.0392},
issn = {2101-6739},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-02-01},
urldate = {2017-02-27},
journal = {Revue de neuropsychologie},
volume = {8},
number = {3},
pages = {192--200},
abstract = {L’amélioration des traitements neuro-oncologiques ces dernières années permettant une survie plus longue des patients atteints de tumeurs cérébrales fait émerger la problématique des effets secondaires à long terme. Parmi ces effets secondaires, les troubles cognitifs sont au premier plan et impactent lourdement la qualité de vie des patients. Leur prise en compte constitue actuellement un challenge majeur en neuro-oncologie et nécessite un effort en recherche clinique ainsi que dans le suivi à moyen et à long terme des patients en pratique courante. Les troubles cognitifs peuvent être causés par la progression tumorale et/ou par la récidive tumorale, mais sont souvent provoqués par la neurotoxicité inhérente aux traitements. L’objectif de cet article est de présenter les troubles cognitifs induits par les traitements anti-tumoraux ainsi que par les traitements concomitants les plus courants en neuro-oncologie. Nous exposons ensuite les principaux biais auxquels se heurte l’évaluation neuropsychologique dans ce contexte et présentons la batterie cognitive proposée par le Grec-Onco, actuellement utilisée dans différents protocoles de recherche clinique.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
L’amélioration des traitements neuro-oncologiques ces dernières années permettant une survie plus longue des patients atteints de tumeurs cérébrales fait émerger la problématique des effets secondaires à long terme. Parmi ces effets secondaires, les troubles cognitifs sont au premier plan et impactent lourdement la qualité de vie des patients. Leur prise en compte constitue actuellement un challenge majeur en neuro-oncologie et nécessite un effort en recherche clinique ainsi que dans le suivi à moyen et à long terme des patients en pratique courante. Les troubles cognitifs peuvent être causés par la progression tumorale et/ou par la récidive tumorale, mais sont souvent provoqués par la neurotoxicité inhérente aux traitements. L’objectif de cet article est de présenter les troubles cognitifs induits par les traitements anti-tumoraux ainsi que par les traitements concomitants les plus courants en neuro-oncologie. Nous exposons ensuite les principaux biais auxquels se heurte l’évaluation neuropsychologique dans ce contexte et présentons la batterie cognitive proposée par le Grec-Onco, actuellement utilisée dans différents protocoles de recherche clinique. |
22. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Martin, Yves; Feuillet, Alexia; Moroni, Christine Exogenous orienting of attention in hearing: a virtual reality paradigm to assess auditory attention in neglect patients Article de journal Dans: Exp Brain Res, 234 (10), p. 2893–2903, 2016, ISSN: 0014-4819, 1432-1106. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{guilbert_exogenous_2016,
title = {Exogenous orienting of attention in hearing: a virtual reality paradigm to assess auditory attention in neglect patients},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Yves Martin and Alexia Feuillet and Christine Moroni},
url = {https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00221-016-4691-8},
doi = {10.1007/s00221-016-4691-8},
issn = {0014-4819, 1432-1106},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-01-01},
urldate = {2017-02-27},
journal = {Exp Brain Res},
volume = {234},
number = {10},
pages = {2893--2903},
abstract = {While mechanisms of orienting attention in unilateral spatial neglect (USN) have frequently been studied in the visual domain, these mechanisms remain relatively unexplored in the auditory domain. Our first goal was to replicate Spence and Driver’s (J Exp Psychol Hum 22:1005–1030, 1994) results with a virtual reality paradigm. This paradigm simulated a 3-dimensional auditory space with headphones. Our second aim was to study auditory profiles of orienting attention in USN. In a first experiment, 18 healthy participants performed an auditory cueing spatial paradigm (either a target-detection task or a target-lateralization task). In a second experiment, 14 right-stroke patients (10 with USN and 4 without USN) performed these two same tasks. As in Spence and Driver’s (J Exp Psychol Hum 22:1005–1030, 1994), our first experiment showed that spatial representations are not utilized for the detection of auditory stimuli. However, during the lateralization task, participants were quicker to detect targets preceded by a spatially congruent cue, which suggests that our paradigm could be suitable for studying orienting attention in hearing. Our second experiment found that patients with USN also needed an explicit spatial task to be sensitive to auditory spatial cueing. In the target-lateralization task, they showed effects lateralized only to one side of space, whereas patients without USN did not. Although our paradigm needs replications to better understand orienting attention impairments in hearing in USN, this study could have implications for the development of clinical tasks that could assess auditory spatial attention in USN syndrome.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
While mechanisms of orienting attention in unilateral spatial neglect (USN) have frequently been studied in the visual domain, these mechanisms remain relatively unexplored in the auditory domain. Our first goal was to replicate Spence and Driver’s (J Exp Psychol Hum 22:1005–1030, 1994) results with a virtual reality paradigm. This paradigm simulated a 3-dimensional auditory space with headphones. Our second aim was to study auditory profiles of orienting attention in USN. In a first experiment, 18 healthy participants performed an auditory cueing spatial paradigm (either a target-detection task or a target-lateralization task). In a second experiment, 14 right-stroke patients (10 with USN and 4 without USN) performed these two same tasks. As in Spence and Driver’s (J Exp Psychol Hum 22:1005–1030, 1994), our first experiment showed that spatial representations are not utilized for the detection of auditory stimuli. However, during the lateralization task, participants were quicker to detect targets preceded by a spatially congruent cue, which suggests that our paradigm could be suitable for studying orienting attention in hearing. Our second experiment found that patients with USN also needed an explicit spatial task to be sensitive to auditory spatial cueing. In the target-lateralization task, they showed effects lateralized only to one side of space, whereas patients without USN did not. Although our paradigm needs replications to better understand orienting attention impairments in hearing in USN, this study could have implications for the development of clinical tasks that could assess auditory spatial attention in USN syndrome. |
23. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Senouci, Latifa; Pontzeele, Sylvain; Martin, Yves; Moroni, Christine Auditory lateralisation deficits in neglect patients Article de journal Dans: Neuropsychologia, 85 , p. 177–183, 2016, ISSN: 0028-3932. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{guilbert_auditory_2016,
title = {Auditory lateralisation deficits in neglect patients},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Latifa Senouci and Sylvain Pontzeele and Yves Martin and Christine Moroni},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.024},
issn = {0028-3932},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-01-01},
journal = {Neuropsychologia},
volume = {85},
pages = {177--183},
abstract = {Although visual deficits due to unilateral spatial neglect (USN) have been frequently described in the literature, fewer studies have been interested in directional hearing impairment in USN. The aim of this study was to explore sound lateralisation deficits in USN. Using a paradigm inspired by Tanaka et al. (1999), interaural time differences (ITD) were presented over headphones to give the illusion of a leftward or a rightward movement of sound. Participants were asked to respond “right” and “left” as soon as possible to indicate whether they heard the sound moving to the right or to the left side of the auditory space. We additionally adopted a single-case method to analyse the performance of 15 patients with right-hemisphere (RH) stroke and added two additional measures to underline sound lateralisation on the left side and on the right side. We included 15 patients with RH stoke (5 with a severe USN, 5 with a mild USN and 5 without USN) and 11 healthy age-matched participants. We expected to replicate findings of abnormal sound lateralisation in USN. However, although a sound lateralisation deficit was observed in USN, two different deficit profiles were identified. Namely, patients with a severe USN seemed to have left sound lateralisation impairment whereas patients with a mild USN seemed to be more influenced by a systematic bias in auditory representation with respect to body meridian axis (egocentric deviation). This latter profile was unexpected as sounds were manipulated with ITD and, thus, would not be perceived as coming from an external source of the head. Future studies should use this paradigm in order to better understand these two distinct profiles.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Although visual deficits due to unilateral spatial neglect (USN) have been frequently described in the literature, fewer studies have been interested in directional hearing impairment in USN. The aim of this study was to explore sound lateralisation deficits in USN. Using a paradigm inspired by Tanaka et al. (1999), interaural time differences (ITD) were presented over headphones to give the illusion of a leftward or a rightward movement of sound. Participants were asked to respond “right” and “left” as soon as possible to indicate whether they heard the sound moving to the right or to the left side of the auditory space. We additionally adopted a single-case method to analyse the performance of 15 patients with right-hemisphere (RH) stroke and added two additional measures to underline sound lateralisation on the left side and on the right side. We included 15 patients with RH stoke (5 with a severe USN, 5 with a mild USN and 5 without USN) and 11 healthy age-matched participants. We expected to replicate findings of abnormal sound lateralisation in USN. However, although a sound lateralisation deficit was observed in USN, two different deficit profiles were identified. Namely, patients with a severe USN seemed to have left sound lateralisation impairment whereas patients with a mild USN seemed to be more influenced by a systematic bias in auditory representation with respect to body meridian axis (egocentric deviation). This latter profile was unexpected as sounds were manipulated with ITD and, thus, would not be perceived as coming from an external source of the head. Future studies should use this paradigm in order to better understand these two distinct profiles. |
24. | Feuillet, Alexia; Moroni, Christine Evaluation des capacités cognitives d’un enfant adopté à l’étranger : Apport et intérêt de la neuropsychologie incollection Dans: 20 cas cliniques en neuropsychologie / sous la direction de Christine Moroni, Pauline Narme, p. 375–395, Dunod, 2016, ISBN: 978-2-10-074023-9. BibTeX @incollection{feuillet_evaluation_2016,
title = {Evaluation des capacités cognitives d’un enfant adopté à l’étranger : Apport et intérêt de la neuropsychologie},
author = {Alexia Feuillet and Christine Moroni},
isbn = {978-2-10-074023-9},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-01-01},
booktitle = {20 cas cliniques en neuropsychologie / sous la direction de Christine Moroni, Pauline Narme},
pages = {375--395},
publisher = {Dunod},
series = {Psycho sup},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {incollection}
}
|
25. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine Évaluation de la latéralisation sonore dans la négligence spatiale unilatérale : cas de Mme B Article de journal Dans: Revue de neuropsychologie, 8 (2), p. 118–125, 2016, ISSN: 2101-6739. Liens | BibTeX @article{guilbert_evaluation_2016-1,
title = {Évaluation de la latéralisation sonore dans la négligence spatiale unilatérale : cas de Mme B},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
url = {http://www.cairn.info/resume.php?ID_ARTICLE=RNE_082_0118},
issn = {2101-6739},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-01-01},
urldate = {2017-02-27},
journal = {Revue de neuropsychologie},
volume = {8},
number = {2},
pages = {118--125},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
26. | Nizzi, Marie-Christine; Belin, Catherine; Maillet, Didier; Moroni, Christine Implication de plusieurs processus cognitifs dans le vécu identitaire des patients Alzheimer et rôle de l’âge subjectif comme marqueur identitaire Article de journal Dans: Gériatrie et Psychologie Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement, 14 (3), p. 341–350, 2016, ISSN: 2115-8789. Liens | BibTeX @article{nizzi_implication_2016,
title = {Implication de plusieurs processus cognitifs dans le vécu identitaire des patients Alzheimer et rôle de l’âge subjectif comme marqueur identitaire},
author = {Marie-Christine Nizzi and Catherine Belin and Didier Maillet and Christine Moroni},
url = {http://www.jle.com/fr/revues/gpn/e-docs/implication_de_plusieurs_processus_cognitifs_dans_le_vecu_identitaire_des_patients_alzheimer_et_role_de_lage_subjectif_comme_marqueur_identitaire_308098/article.phtml?tab=texte},
doi = {10.1684/pnv.2016.0624},
issn = {2115-8789},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-01-01},
urldate = {2017-02-27},
journal = {Gériatrie et Psychologie Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement},
volume = {14},
number = {3},
pages = {341--350},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2015
|
27. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine La pratique musicale comme outil de rééducation de la négligence spatiale unilatérale 5500 - 5599 Conférence 2015. BibTeX @conference{Guilbert2015,
title = {La pratique musicale comme outil de rééducation de la négligence spatiale unilatérale},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-12-01},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
28. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine Midpoint auditif: évaluation de la négligence spatiale unilatérale en modalité auditive. 5500 - 5599 Conférence 2015. BibTeX @conference{guilbert_midpoint_2015,
title = {Midpoint auditif: évaluation de la négligence spatiale unilatérale en modalité auditive.},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-05-01},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
29. | Caroline, Marks; Ikram, Bouacha; Sabine, Defoort; Daniele, Basset; Christine, Moroni Principes de réalisation du champ visuel attentionnel et élaboration de normes Article de journal Dans: 38 (6), p. 486–492, 2015, ISSN: 0181-5512. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{caroline_principes_2015,
title = {Principes de réalisation du champ visuel attentionnel et élaboration de normes},
author = {Marks Caroline and Bouacha Ikram and Defoort Sabine and Basset Daniele and Moroni Christine},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0181551215001114},
doi = {10.1016/j.jfo.2014.11.014},
issn = {0181-5512},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-05-01},
urldate = {2016-05-03},
volume = {38},
number = {6},
pages = {486--492},
abstract = {RésuméIntroduction Le champ visuel attentionnel est un examen permettant de prendre en compte l’influence de facteurs attentionnels lors de l’évaluation du champ visuel. L’objectif de notre étude était de recueillir des données contrôles adaptées à notre procédure utilisée en pratique clinique, lors des consultations d’évaluation à la reprise de la conduite automobile. Patients et méthodes Cinquante-deux participants témoins âgés de 19 à 69 ans, sans antécédent ophtalmologique ou neurologique, ont réalisé notre adaptation de la procédure de champ visuel attentionnel. Le nombre d’omissions de cibles centrales et latérales et les temps de réponses ont été mesurés au cours de trois tâches attentionnelles. Résultats Le nombre moyen d’omissions et les temps moyens de détection des cibles augmentent progressivement au cours des trois tâches. L’âge et le sexe sont des facteurs influençant de manière significative les temps de détection des cibles, les hommes et les sujets jeunes (de 19 à 29 ans) étant plus rapides. Conclusion Le champ visuel attentionnel est reconnu comme un outil fiable dans le cadre de l’évaluation de la reprise de la conduite automobile. Pour permettre une utilisation adaptée à notre pratique clinique, il était indispensable d’élaborer des normes pour interpréter les performances attentionnelles des patients ayant une atteinte de leur champ visuel. SummaryPurpose The useful field of view is a test which takes into account the influence of patient attention during the evaluation of the visual field. This study aims to generate normative data for an adaptation of the useful field of view (UFOV) test. These normative data are essential to judge, in a clinical setting, whether patients’ performance is normal or not across demographically similar peers, in order to advise whether or not to resume driving after a brain injury (traumatic brain injury, stroke or cerebral tumour). Patients and methods This study examined demographic influences on an UFOV adaptation in a sample of 52 control participants (17 males and 35 females, aged 19 to 69) with no prior ophthalmologic or neurologic history. This adaptation used three visual attention tasks. In a simple task (ST), the participant had to detect, as fast as (s)he could, a single target in a visual display; in a double task (DT,) (s)he had to detect both a central and a lateral target in a double task with visual distractions present on the screen. The number of missed targets and the time needed to detect them are measured. Results Time to detect target was found to differ by gender and by age. Men and young people (from 19 to 29 years) are faster at detecting central and lateral targets. However, no demographic influence was observed on the number of missed targets. Conclusions A normative table for this French UFOV adaptation is provided. This will allow clinicians to compare patient performance with similar peers and may help in identifying persons who would benefit from training on a driving simulator or having a road test with a driving-school.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
RésuméIntroduction Le champ visuel attentionnel est un examen permettant de prendre en compte l’influence de facteurs attentionnels lors de l’évaluation du champ visuel. L’objectif de notre étude était de recueillir des données contrôles adaptées à notre procédure utilisée en pratique clinique, lors des consultations d’évaluation à la reprise de la conduite automobile. Patients et méthodes Cinquante-deux participants témoins âgés de 19 à 69 ans, sans antécédent ophtalmologique ou neurologique, ont réalisé notre adaptation de la procédure de champ visuel attentionnel. Le nombre d’omissions de cibles centrales et latérales et les temps de réponses ont été mesurés au cours de trois tâches attentionnelles. Résultats Le nombre moyen d’omissions et les temps moyens de détection des cibles augmentent progressivement au cours des trois tâches. L’âge et le sexe sont des facteurs influençant de manière significative les temps de détection des cibles, les hommes et les sujets jeunes (de 19 à 29 ans) étant plus rapides. Conclusion Le champ visuel attentionnel est reconnu comme un outil fiable dans le cadre de l’évaluation de la reprise de la conduite automobile. Pour permettre une utilisation adaptée à notre pratique clinique, il était indispensable d’élaborer des normes pour interpréter les performances attentionnelles des patients ayant une atteinte de leur champ visuel. SummaryPurpose The useful field of view is a test which takes into account the influence of patient attention during the evaluation of the visual field. This study aims to generate normative data for an adaptation of the useful field of view (UFOV) test. These normative data are essential to judge, in a clinical setting, whether patients’ performance is normal or not across demographically similar peers, in order to advise whether or not to resume driving after a brain injury (traumatic brain injury, stroke or cerebral tumour). Patients and methods This study examined demographic influences on an UFOV adaptation in a sample of 52 control participants (17 males and 35 females, aged 19 to 69) with no prior ophthalmologic or neurologic history. This adaptation used three visual attention tasks. In a simple task (ST), the participant had to detect, as fast as (s)he could, a single target in a visual display; in a double task (DT,) (s)he had to detect both a central and a lateral target in a double task with visual distractions present on the screen. The number of missed targets and the time needed to detect them are measured. Results Time to detect target was found to differ by gender and by age. Men and young people (from 19 to 29 years) are faster at detecting central and lateral targets. However, no demographic influence was observed on the number of missed targets. Conclusions A normative table for this French UFOV adaptation is provided. This will allow clinicians to compare patient performance with similar peers and may help in identifying persons who would benefit from training on a driving simulator or having a road test with a driving-school. |
30. | Defoort, Sabine; Basset, Daniele; Marks, Caroline; Bouacha, Ikram; Moroni, Christine Principes de réalisation du champ visuel attentionnel et élaboration de normes. Article de journal Dans: Journal Français d’Ophtalmologie, 38 , p. 486–492, 2015. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{Defoort2015,
title = {Principes de réalisation du champ visuel attentionnel et élaboration de normes.},
author = {Sabine Defoort and Daniele Basset and Caroline Marks and Ikram Bouacha and Christine Moroni},
doi = {j.jfo.2014.11.014},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-01-01},
journal = {Journal Français d’Ophtalmologie},
volume = {38},
pages = {486--492},
abstract = {Résumé Introduction. — Le champ visuel attentionnel est un examen permettant de prendre en compte l’influence de facteurs attentionnels lors de l’évaluation du champ visuel. L’objectif de notre étude était de recueillir des données contrôles adaptées à notre procédure utilisée en pratique clinique, lors des consultations d’évaluation à la reprise de la conduite automobile. Patients et méthodes. — Cinquante-deux participants témoins âgés de 19 à 69 ans, sans anté- cédent ophtalmologique ou neurologique, ont réalisé notre adaptation de la procédure de champ visuel attentionnel. Le nombre d’omissions de cibles centrales et latérales et les temps de réponses ont été mesurés au cours de trois tâches attentionnelles. Résultats. — Le nombre moyen d’omissions et les temps moyens de détection des cibles aug- mentent progressivement au cours des trois tâches. L’âge et le sexe sont des facteurs influen ̧cant de manière significative les temps de détection des cibles, les hommes et les sujets jeunes (de 19 à 29 ans) étant plus rapides. Conclusion. — Le champ visuel attentionnel est reconnu comme un outil fiable dans le cadre de l’évaluation de la reprise de la conduite automobile. Pour permettre une utilisation adaptée à notre pratique clinique, il était indispensable d’élaborer des normes pour interpréter les performances attentionnelles des patients ayant une atteinte de leur champ visuel. © 2015 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Résumé Introduction. — Le champ visuel attentionnel est un examen permettant de prendre en compte l’influence de facteurs attentionnels lors de l’évaluation du champ visuel. L’objectif de notre étude était de recueillir des données contrôles adaptées à notre procédure utilisée en pratique clinique, lors des consultations d’évaluation à la reprise de la conduite automobile. Patients et méthodes. — Cinquante-deux participants témoins âgés de 19 à 69 ans, sans anté- cédent ophtalmologique ou neurologique, ont réalisé notre adaptation de la procédure de champ visuel attentionnel. Le nombre d’omissions de cibles centrales et latérales et les temps de réponses ont été mesurés au cours de trois tâches attentionnelles. Résultats. — Le nombre moyen d’omissions et les temps moyens de détection des cibles aug- mentent progressivement au cours des trois tâches. L’âge et le sexe sont des facteurs influen ̧cant de manière significative les temps de détection des cibles, les hommes et les sujets jeunes (de 19 à 29 ans) étant plus rapides. Conclusion. — Le champ visuel attentionnel est reconnu comme un outil fiable dans le cadre de l’évaluation de la reprise de la conduite automobile. Pour permettre une utilisation adaptée à notre pratique clinique, il était indispensable d’élaborer des normes pour interpréter les performances attentionnelles des patients ayant une atteinte de leur champ visuel. © 2015 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés. |
31. | Taillia, Hervé; Léger, Isabelle; Christine, Moroni; Mathieu, Boone; C, Belin; Didier, Maillet; M, Lange La batterie du Grec-Onco (Groupe de Réflexion sur les Evaluations Cognitives en ONCOlogie). Gremoire 2, Tests et échelles des maladies neurologiques avec symptomatologie cognitive. incollection Dans: pp Sous la direction de Laurence Hugonot-Diener Catherine Thomas-Antérion et François Sellal. De Boeck-Solal. Paris, : 33-37 (Ed.): 2015. BibTeX @incollection{taillia_batterie_2015,
title = {La batterie du Grec-Onco (Groupe de Réflexion sur les Evaluations Cognitives en ONCOlogie). Gremoire 2, Tests et échelles des maladies neurologiques avec symptomatologie cognitive.},
author = {Hervé Taillia and Isabelle Léger and Moroni Christine and Boone Mathieu and Belin C and Maillet Didier and Lange M},
editor = {Sous la direction de Laurence Hugonot-Diener, Catherine Thomas-Antérion et François Sellal. De Boeck-Solal. Paris, pp : 33-37.},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-01-01},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {incollection}
}
|
2014
|
32. | Chartaux-Danjou, Laurence; Moroni, Christine; Delecroix, Hélène Quels impacts dans le travail pour quels troubles cognitifs ? Article de journal Dans: Archives des maladies professionnelles et de l’environnement, 5 (72), p. 530–531, 2014. Liens | BibTeX @article{Chartaux-Danjou2014,
title = {Quels impacts dans le travail pour quels troubles cognitifs ?},
author = {Laurence Chartaux-Danjou and Christine Moroni and Hélène Delecroix},
doi = {10.1016/j.admp.2014.07.041},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-11-01},
journal = {Archives des maladies professionnelles et de l’environnement},
volume = {5},
number = {72},
pages = {530--531},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
33. | Leclercq, Eugénie; Cabaret, Maryline; Guilbert, Alma; Jougleux, Caroline; Vermersch, Patrick; Moroni, Christine The influence of age and illness duration on cognitive impairment in aging patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RR-MS). Article de journal Dans: Gériatrie et psychologie neuropsychiatrie du vieillissement, 12 (3), p. 331–8, 2014, ISSN: 2115-7863. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{leclercq_[influence_2014,
title = {The influence of age and illness duration on cognitive impairment in aging patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RR-MS).},
author = {Eugénie Leclercq and Maryline Cabaret and Alma Guilbert and Caroline Jougleux and Patrick Vermersch and Christine Moroni},
url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25245320},
doi = {10.1684/pnv.2014.0482},
issn = {2115-7863},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
journal = {Gériatrie et psychologie neuropsychiatrie du vieillissement},
volume = {12},
number = {3},
pages = {331--8},
abstract = {The aim of this study was to dissociate age and duration of illness effects on cognitive impairment of patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Cognitive impairment among patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) is well known. However, few studies were devoted to assess the respective role of disease duration and age on cognitive functions in MS patients. Therefore, two studies were carried out on relapsing-remitting MS (RR-MS) patients using some tests of the BCcogSEP–a French test battery evaluating cognitive functions in MS. The cognitive deficits of RR-MS patients aged 50 years and over and whose symptoms had been present for more than 20 years were more severe than those of MS patients with a shorter illness duration (less than 10 years) or matched-age control participants. The more impaired cognitive functions were information-processing speed, episodic memory, verbal fluency and attention. On the other hand, cognitive performances of young RR-MS patients were similar to those of older RR-MS patients when all patients had the same illness duration (8 years in this study). Older patients even achieved better performance than younger ones on verbal fluency. This can be partly explained by the theory of cognitive reserve, as reported in previous cognitive aging studies. In RR-MS patients, the influence of illness duration seems to be a predominant factor in the development of cognitive impairment.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
The aim of this study was to dissociate age and duration of illness effects on cognitive impairment of patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Cognitive impairment among patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) is well known. However, few studies were devoted to assess the respective role of disease duration and age on cognitive functions in MS patients. Therefore, two studies were carried out on relapsing-remitting MS (RR-MS) patients using some tests of the BCcogSEP–a French test battery evaluating cognitive functions in MS. The cognitive deficits of RR-MS patients aged 50 years and over and whose symptoms had been present for more than 20 years were more severe than those of MS patients with a shorter illness duration (less than 10 years) or matched-age control participants. The more impaired cognitive functions were information-processing speed, episodic memory, verbal fluency and attention. On the other hand, cognitive performances of young RR-MS patients were similar to those of older RR-MS patients when all patients had the same illness duration (8 years in this study). Older patients even achieved better performance than younger ones on verbal fluency. This can be partly explained by the theory of cognitive reserve, as reported in previous cognitive aging studies. In RR-MS patients, the influence of illness duration seems to be a predominant factor in the development of cognitive impairment. |
34. | Demeulemeester, Morgane; Moroni, Christine; Kochman, Frederick; Thomas, Pierre; Jardri, Renaud Hallucinations et cognition : une modélisation au service de notre pratique en neuropsychologie. Article de journal Dans: Revue de Neuropsychologie, 6 (2), p. 117–128, 2014. Liens | BibTeX @article{demeulemeester_hallucinations_2014,
title = {Hallucinations et cognition : une modélisation au service de notre pratique en neuropsychologie.},
author = {Morgane Demeulemeester and Christine Moroni and Frederick Kochman and Pierre Thomas and Renaud Jardri},
doi = {10.1684/nrp.2014.0298},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
journal = {Revue de Neuropsychologie},
volume = {6},
number = {2},
pages = {117--128},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
35. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine Hearing and music in unilateral spatial neglect neuro-rehabilitation Article de journal Dans: Front Psychol, 5 , p. 1503, 2014, ISSN: 1664-1078. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{guilbert_hearing_2014,
title = {Hearing and music in unilateral spatial neglect neuro-rehabilitation},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01503},
issn = {1664-1078},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
journal = {Front Psychol},
volume = {5},
pages = {1503},
abstract = {Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) is an attention deficit in the contralesional side of space which occurs after a cerebral stroke, mainly located in the right hemisphere. USN patients are disabled in all daily activities. USN is an important negative prognostic factor of functional recovery and of socio-professional reinsertion. Thus, patient rehabilitation is a major challenge. As this deficit has been described in many sensory modalities (including hearing), many sensory and poly-sensory rehabilitation methods have been proposed to USN patients. They are mainly based on visual, tactile modalities and on motor abilities. However, these methods appear to be quite task-specific and difficult to transfer to functional activities. Very few studies have focused on the hearing modality and even fewer studies have been conducted in music as a way of improving spatial attention. Therefore, more research on such retraining needs is neccessary in order to make reliable conclusions on its efficiency in long-term rehabilitation. Nevertheless, some evidence suggests that music could be a promising tool to enhance spatial attention and to rehabilitate USN patients. In fact, music is a material closely linked to space, involving common anatomical and functional networks. The present paper aims firstly at briefly reviewing the different procedures of sensory retraining proposed in USN, including auditory retraining, and their limits. Secondly, it aims to present the recent scientific evidence that makes music a good candidate for USN patients' neuro-rehabilitation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) is an attention deficit in the contralesional side of space which occurs after a cerebral stroke, mainly located in the right hemisphere. USN patients are disabled in all daily activities. USN is an important negative prognostic factor of functional recovery and of socio-professional reinsertion. Thus, patient rehabilitation is a major challenge. As this deficit has been described in many sensory modalities (including hearing), many sensory and poly-sensory rehabilitation methods have been proposed to USN patients. They are mainly based on visual, tactile modalities and on motor abilities. However, these methods appear to be quite task-specific and difficult to transfer to functional activities. Very few studies have focused on the hearing modality and even fewer studies have been conducted in music as a way of improving spatial attention. Therefore, more research on such retraining needs is neccessary in order to make reliable conclusions on its efficiency in long-term rehabilitation. Nevertheless, some evidence suggests that music could be a promising tool to enhance spatial attention and to rehabilitate USN patients. In fact, music is a material closely linked to space, involving common anatomical and functional networks. The present paper aims firstly at briefly reviewing the different procedures of sensory retraining proposed in USN, including auditory retraining, and their limits. Secondly, it aims to present the recent scientific evidence that makes music a good candidate for USN patients' neuro-rehabilitation. |
36. | Boucart, Muriel; Calais, Gauthier; Lenoble, Quentin; Moroni, Christine; Pasquier, Florence Differential processing of natural scenes in posterior cortical atrophy and in Alzheimer's disease, as measured with a saccade choice task Article de journal Dans: Front Integr Neurosci, 8 , p. 60, 2014, ISSN: 1662-5145. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{boucart_differential_2014,
title = {Differential processing of natural scenes in posterior cortical atrophy and in Alzheimer's disease, as measured with a saccade choice task},
author = {Muriel Boucart and Gauthier Calais and Quentin Lenoble and Christine Moroni and Florence Pasquier},
doi = {10.3389/fnint.2014.00060},
issn = {1662-5145},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
journal = {Front Integr Neurosci},
volume = {8},
pages = {60},
abstract = {Atrophy of the medial temporal lobe structures that support scene perception and the binding of an object to its context (i.e., the hippocampus and the parahippocampal cortex) appears early in the course of Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, few studies have investigated scene perception in people with AD. Here, we assessed the ability to find a target object within a natural scene in people with AD and in people with posterior cortical atrophy (PCA, a variant of AD). Pairs of color photographs were displayed on the left and right of a fixation cross for 1 s. In separate blocks of trials, participants were asked to categorize the target (an animal) by either moving their eyes toward the photograph containing the target (the saccadic choice task) or pressing a key corresponding to the target's location (the manual choice task). Isolated objects and objects within scenes were studied in both tasks. Participants with PCA were more impaired in detection of a target within a scene than participants with AD. The latter's performance pattern was more similar to that of age-matched controls in terms of accuracy, saccade latencies and the benefit gained from contextual information. Participants with PCA benefited less from contextual information in both the saccade and the manual choice tasks-suggesting that people with posterior brain lesions have impairments in figure/ground segregation and are more sensitive to object crowding.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Atrophy of the medial temporal lobe structures that support scene perception and the binding of an object to its context (i.e., the hippocampus and the parahippocampal cortex) appears early in the course of Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, few studies have investigated scene perception in people with AD. Here, we assessed the ability to find a target object within a natural scene in people with AD and in people with posterior cortical atrophy (PCA, a variant of AD). Pairs of color photographs were displayed on the left and right of a fixation cross for 1 s. In separate blocks of trials, participants were asked to categorize the target (an animal) by either moving their eyes toward the photograph containing the target (the saccadic choice task) or pressing a key corresponding to the target's location (the manual choice task). Isolated objects and objects within scenes were studied in both tasks. Participants with PCA were more impaired in detection of a target within a scene than participants with AD. The latter's performance pattern was more similar to that of age-matched controls in terms of accuracy, saccade latencies and the benefit gained from contextual information. Participants with PCA benefited less from contextual information in both the saccade and the manual choice tasks-suggesting that people with posterior brain lesions have impairments in figure/ground segregation and are more sensitive to object crowding. |
37. | Haj, Mohamad El; Moroni, Christine; Luyat, Marion; Omigie, Diana; Allain, Philippe To what extent does destination recall induce episodic reliving? Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease Article de journal Dans: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 36 (2), p. 127–136, 2014, ISSN: 1380-3395. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{haj_what_2014,
title = {To what extent does destination recall induce episodic reliving? Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease},
author = {Mohamad El Haj and Christine Moroni and Marion Luyat and Diana Omigie and Philippe Allain},
url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2013.869309},
doi = {10.1080/13803395.2013.869309},
issn = {1380-3395},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
urldate = {2015-03-26},
journal = {Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology},
volume = {36},
number = {2},
pages = {127--136},
abstract = {We compared destination memory to source memory in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), as the latter type of memory is believed to be severely deteriorated in AD. Control participants and AD patients were tested on two conditions, both of which had a study phase and a recognition phase. In the study phase of the first condition, participants had to tell a set of facts to the faces of a set of celebrities (destination memory). In the study phase of the second condition, they had to receive a different set of facts from a different set of celebrity faces (source memory). During the recognition phase, participants had to indicate, for destination memory, whether they had previously told a given fact to a given face (yes) or not (no) and, for source memory, whether they had previously received a given fact from a given face (yes) or not (no). In both conditions, they were asked to choose between “remember” or “know” options when answering “yes.” AD patients showed reliable difficulties in destination recall, accompanied by a significant decrease in the number of “remember” responses they gave. AD-related destination memory decline may be attributed to the perturbation of episodic memory and its autonoetic reliving. The potential neural bases of this decline are discussed in terms of hippocampal failures.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
We compared destination memory to source memory in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), as the latter type of memory is believed to be severely deteriorated in AD. Control participants and AD patients were tested on two conditions, both of which had a study phase and a recognition phase. In the study phase of the first condition, participants had to tell a set of facts to the faces of a set of celebrities (destination memory). In the study phase of the second condition, they had to receive a different set of facts from a different set of celebrity faces (source memory). During the recognition phase, participants had to indicate, for destination memory, whether they had previously told a given fact to a given face (yes) or not (no) and, for source memory, whether they had previously received a given fact from a given face (yes) or not (no). In both conditions, they were asked to choose between “remember” or “know” options when answering “yes.” AD patients showed reliable difficulties in destination recall, accompanied by a significant decrease in the number of “remember” responses they gave. AD-related destination memory decline may be attributed to the perturbation of episodic memory and its autonoetic reliving. The potential neural bases of this decline are discussed in terms of hippocampal failures. |
38. | Leclercq, Eugénie; Cabaret, Maryline; Guilbert, Alma; Jougleux, Caroline; Vermersch, Patrick; Moroni, Christine Influence de la durée d’évolution de la maladie et de l’âge sur les troubles cognitifs de patients âgés atteints d’une sclérose en plaques de forme rémittente (SEP-RR) Article de journal Dans: Gériatrie et Psychologie Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement, 12 (3), p. 331–338, 2014, ISSN: 2115-8789. Liens | BibTeX @article{leclercq_influence_2014,
title = {Influence de la durée d’évolution de la maladie et de l’âge sur les troubles cognitifs de patients âgés atteints d’une sclérose en plaques de forme rémittente (SEP-RR)},
author = {Eugénie Leclercq and Maryline Cabaret and Alma Guilbert and Caroline Jougleux and Patrick Vermersch and Christine Moroni},
url = {http://www.jle.com/fr/revues/gpn/e-docs/influence_de_la_duree_devolution_de_la_maladie_et_de_lage_sur_les_troubles_cognitifs_de_patients_ages_atteints_dune_sclerose_en_plaques_de_forme_remittente_sep_rr__302375/article.phtml?tab=texte},
doi = {10.1684/pnv.2014.0482},
issn = {2115-8789},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
urldate = {2017-08-28},
journal = {Gériatrie et Psychologie Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement},
volume = {12},
number = {3},
pages = {331--338},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
39. | Haj, Mohamad El; Omigie, Diana; Moroni, Christine Time reproduction during high and low attentional tasks in Alzheimer’s Disease “A watched kettle never boils” Article de journal Dans: Brain and Cognition, 88 , p. 1–5, 2014, ISSN: 02782626. Liens | BibTeX @article{el_haj_time_2014,
title = {Time reproduction during high and low attentional tasks in Alzheimer’s Disease “A watched kettle never boils”},
author = {Mohamad El Haj and Diana Omigie and Christine Moroni},
url = {http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0278262614000645},
doi = {10.1016/j.bandc.2014.04.002},
issn = {02782626},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
urldate = {2017-08-28},
journal = {Brain and Cognition},
volume = {88},
pages = {1--5},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
2013
|
40. | Guilbert, Alma; Feuillet, Alexia; Clément, Sylvain; Moroni, Christine Orientations exogène et endogène de l'attention en modalités visuelle et auditive dans la négligence spatiale unilatérale 5500 - 5599 Conférence 2013. BibTeX @conference{guilbert_orientations_2013,
title = {Orientations exogène et endogène de l'attention en modalités visuelle et auditive dans la négligence spatiale unilatérale},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Alexia Feuillet and Sylvain Clément and Christine Moroni},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-12-01},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
41. | Guilbert, Alma; Clément, Sylvain; Séverine, Samson; Moroni, Christine Musical practice as rehabilitation method in unilateral spatial neglect 5500 - 5599 Conférence 2013. BibTeX @conference{Guilbert2013,
title = {Musical practice as rehabilitation method in unilateral spatial neglect},
author = {Alma Guilbert and Sylvain Clément and Samson Séverine and Christine Moroni},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-09-01},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {conference}
}
|
42. | Boucart, Muriel; Moroni, Christine; Szaffarczyk, Sebastien; Tran, Thi Ha Chau Implicit processing of scene context in macular degeneration. Article de journal Dans: Investigative ophthalmology & visual science, 54 (3), p. 1950–7, 2013, ISSN: 1552-5783. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{boucart_implicit_2013,
title = {Implicit processing of scene context in macular degeneration.},
author = {Muriel Boucart and Christine Moroni and Sebastien Szaffarczyk and Thi Ha Chau Tran},
url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23425698},
doi = {10.1167/iovs.12-9680},
issn = {1552-5783},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-03-01},
journal = {Investigative ophthalmology & visual science},
volume = {54},
number = {3},
pages = {1950--7},
abstract = {PURPOSE: For normally sighted people, there is a general consensus that objects that appear in a congruent context (e.g., a hair dryer in a bathroom) are processed more accurately and/or more quickly than objects in an incongruent context (e.g., a hair dryer in a corn field). We investigated whether people with AMD, who have impairments in recognizing objects embedded in complex scenes, can nevertheless take advantage of contextual information for object detection. METHODS: TWENTY-TWO PEOPLE WITH AMD AND 18 AGE-MATCHED, NORMALLY SIGHTED CONTROLS TOOK PART IN THE STUDY. THEY WERE TESTED IN TWO TASKS: (1) an object detection task in which a foreground target object was set within a congruent background or an incongruent background, with no information being given to the participants as to the relationship between the target and its background, and (2) a task in which the participant had to explicitly state whether or not the foreground object was congruent with its background. A go/no-go paradigm was used in both tasks (i.e., a key press when the target is present and no key press when it is absent). The same participants, stimuli, and presentation conditions were used in both tasks. RESULTS: In the context task, the people with AMD exhibited higher sensitivity when the target object was consistent with its background; however, they performed no better than chance in the explicit task. Normally sighted controls benefited from the congruent context in both tasks. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that when central vision is impaired (as in AMD), the contextual information captured by peripheral vision provides cues for object categorization.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
PURPOSE: For normally sighted people, there is a general consensus that objects that appear in a congruent context (e.g., a hair dryer in a bathroom) are processed more accurately and/or more quickly than objects in an incongruent context (e.g., a hair dryer in a corn field). We investigated whether people with AMD, who have impairments in recognizing objects embedded in complex scenes, can nevertheless take advantage of contextual information for object detection. METHODS: TWENTY-TWO PEOPLE WITH AMD AND 18 AGE-MATCHED, NORMALLY SIGHTED CONTROLS TOOK PART IN THE STUDY. THEY WERE TESTED IN TWO TASKS: (1) an object detection task in which a foreground target object was set within a congruent background or an incongruent background, with no information being given to the participants as to the relationship between the target and its background, and (2) a task in which the participant had to explicitly state whether or not the foreground object was congruent with its background. A go/no-go paradigm was used in both tasks (i.e., a key press when the target is present and no key press when it is absent). The same participants, stimuli, and presentation conditions were used in both tasks. RESULTS: In the context task, the people with AMD exhibited higher sensitivity when the target object was consistent with its background; however, they performed no better than chance in the explicit task. Normally sighted controls benefited from the congruent context in both tasks. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that when central vision is impaired (as in AMD), the contextual information captured by peripheral vision provides cues for object categorization. |
43. | Delbeuck, Xavier; Debachy, Brigitte; Pasquier, Florence; Moroni, Christine Action and noun fluency testing to distinguish between Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies Article de journal Dans: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35 (3), p. 259–268, 2013, ISSN: 1380-3395. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{delbeuck_action_2013,
title = {Action and noun fluency testing to distinguish between Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies},
author = {Xavier Delbeuck and Brigitte Debachy and Florence Pasquier and Christine Moroni},
url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2013.763907},
doi = {10.1080/13803395.2013.763907},
issn = {1380-3395},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-03-01},
urldate = {2015-03-26},
journal = {Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology},
volume = {35},
number = {3},
pages = {259--268},
abstract = {The objective of the present study was to establish whether performance in an action fluency task is of value in the differential diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). After collecting normative data on performance in an action fluency task and a conventional animal fluency task in a cohort of French-speaking healthy controls, we assessed AD and DLB patients. Only the action fluency score differed significantly between the two demented groups, with DLB patients performing worse than AD patients. However, a composite action and animal fluency score was found to be more effective for discriminating between these two groups.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
The objective of the present study was to establish whether performance in an action fluency task is of value in the differential diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). After collecting normative data on performance in an action fluency task and a conventional animal fluency task in a cohort of French-speaking healthy controls, we assessed AD and DLB patients. Only the action fluency score differed significantly between the two demented groups, with DLB patients performing worse than AD patients. However, a composite action and animal fluency score was found to be more effective for discriminating between these two groups. |
44. | Godefroy, O; Leclercq, C; Bugnicourt, J -M; Roussel, M; Moroni, C; Quaglino, V; Beaunieux, H; Taillia, H; Nédélec-Ciceri, C; Bonnin, C; Thomas-Anterion, C; Varvat, J; Aboulafia-Brakha, T; Assal, F Neuropsychological assessment and cerebral vascular disease: the new standards. Article de journal Dans: Revue neurologique, 169 (10), p. 779–85, 2013, ISSN: 0035-3787. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{godefroy_neuropsychological_2013,
title = {Neuropsychological assessment and cerebral vascular disease: the new standards.},
author = {O Godefroy and C Leclercq and J -M Bugnicourt and M Roussel and C Moroni and V Quaglino and H Beaunieux and H Taillia and C Nédélec-Ciceri and C Bonnin and C Thomas-Anterion and J Varvat and T Aboulafia-Brakha and F Assal},
url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23999023},
doi = {10.1016/j.neurol.2013.07.009},
issn = {0035-3787},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-01-01},
journal = {Revue neurologique},
volume = {169},
number = {10},
pages = {779--85},
abstract = {Vascular cognitive impairment (VCI) includes vascular dementia (VaD), vascular mild cognitive impairment (VaMCI) and mixed dementia. In clinical practice, VCI concerns patients referred for clinical stroke or cognitive complaint. To improve the characterization of VCI and to refine its diagnostic criteria, an international group has elaborated a new standardized evaluation battery of clinical, cognitive, behavioral and neuroradiological data which now constitutes the reference battery. The adaption of the battery for French-speaking subjects is reported as well as preliminary results of the on-going validation study of the GRECOG-VASC group [Clinical Trial NCT01339195]. The diagnostic accuracy of various screening tests is reviewed and showed an overall sub-optimal sensitivity (textless0.8). Thus, the general recommendation is to perform systematically a comprehensive assessment in stroke patients at risk of VCI. Furthermore,the use of a structured interview has been shown to increase the detection of dementia. In addition to the well known NINDS-AIREN criteria of VaD, criteria of VCI have been recently proposed which are based on the demonstration of a cognitive disorder by neuropsychological testing and either history of clinical stroke or presence of vascular lesion by neuroimaging suggestive of a link between cognitive impairment and vascular disease. A memory deficit is no longer required for the diagnosis of VaD as it is based on the cognitive decline concerning two or more domains that affect activities of daily living. Both VaMCI and VaD are classified as probable or possible. These new criteria have yet to be validated. Considerable uncertainties remain regarding the determinant of VCI, and especially the lesion amount inducing VCI and VaD. The interaction between lesion amount and its location is currently re-examined using recent techniques for the analysis of MRI data. The high frequency of associated Alzheimer pathology is now assessable in vivo using amyloid imaging. The first studies showed that about a third of patients with VaD due to small vessel disease or with poststroke dementia have amyloid PET imaging suggestive of AD. These new techniques will examine the interaction between vascular lesions and promotion of amyloid deposition. Although results of these on-going studies will be available in few years, these data indicate that efforts should be done in clinical practice to reduce underdiagnosis of VCI; VCI should be examined using a specific protocol which will be fully normalized soon for French-speaking patients; the sub-optimal sensitivity of screening tests prompts to use a structured interview to grade Rankin scale and to perform systematically a comprehensive assessment in stroke patients at risk of VCI; poststroke dementia occurring after 3 months poststroke may be preventable by treatment of modifiable vascular risk factors and secondary prevention of stroke recurrence according to recent recommendations.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Vascular cognitive impairment (VCI) includes vascular dementia (VaD), vascular mild cognitive impairment (VaMCI) and mixed dementia. In clinical practice, VCI concerns patients referred for clinical stroke or cognitive complaint. To improve the characterization of VCI and to refine its diagnostic criteria, an international group has elaborated a new standardized evaluation battery of clinical, cognitive, behavioral and neuroradiological data which now constitutes the reference battery. The adaption of the battery for French-speaking subjects is reported as well as preliminary results of the on-going validation study of the GRECOG-VASC group [Clinical Trial NCT01339195]. The diagnostic accuracy of various screening tests is reviewed and showed an overall sub-optimal sensitivity (textless0.8). Thus, the general recommendation is to perform systematically a comprehensive assessment in stroke patients at risk of VCI. Furthermore,the use of a structured interview has been shown to increase the detection of dementia. In addition to the well known NINDS-AIREN criteria of VaD, criteria of VCI have been recently proposed which are based on the demonstration of a cognitive disorder by neuropsychological testing and either history of clinical stroke or presence of vascular lesion by neuroimaging suggestive of a link between cognitive impairment and vascular disease. A memory deficit is no longer required for the diagnosis of VaD as it is based on the cognitive decline concerning two or more domains that affect activities of daily living. Both VaMCI and VaD are classified as probable or possible. These new criteria have yet to be validated. Considerable uncertainties remain regarding the determinant of VCI, and especially the lesion amount inducing VCI and VaD. The interaction between lesion amount and its location is currently re-examined using recent techniques for the analysis of MRI data. The high frequency of associated Alzheimer pathology is now assessable in vivo using amyloid imaging. The first studies showed that about a third of patients with VaD due to small vessel disease or with poststroke dementia have amyloid PET imaging suggestive of AD. These new techniques will examine the interaction between vascular lesions and promotion of amyloid deposition. Although results of these on-going studies will be available in few years, these data indicate that efforts should be done in clinical practice to reduce underdiagnosis of VCI; VCI should be examined using a specific protocol which will be fully normalized soon for French-speaking patients; the sub-optimal sensitivity of screening tests prompts to use a structured interview to grade Rankin scale and to perform systematically a comprehensive assessment in stroke patients at risk of VCI; poststroke dementia occurring after 3 months poststroke may be preventable by treatment of modifiable vascular risk factors and secondary prevention of stroke recurrence according to recent recommendations. |
45. | Haj, Mohamad El; Moroni, Christine; Samson, Séverine; Fasotti, Luciano; Allain, Philippe Prospective and retrospective time perception are related to mental time travel: Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease Article de journal Dans: Brain and Cognition, 83 (1), p. 45–51, 2013, ISSN: 0278-2626. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{haj_prospective_2013,
title = {Prospective and retrospective time perception are related to mental time travel: Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease},
author = {Mohamad El Haj and Christine Moroni and Séverine Samson and Luciano Fasotti and Philippe Allain},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0278262613000882},
doi = {10.1016/j.bandc.2013.06.008},
issn = {0278-2626},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-01-01},
urldate = {2015-03-26},
journal = {Brain and Cognition},
volume = {83},
number = {1},
pages = {45--51},
abstract = {Unlike prospective time perception paradigms, in which participants are aware that they have to estimate forthcoming time, little is known about retrospective time perception in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Our paper addresses this shortcoming by comparing prospective and retrospective time estimation in younger adults, older adults, and AD patients. In four prospective tasks (lasting 30 s, 60 s, 90 s, or 120 s) participants were asked to read a series of numbers and to provide a verbal estimation of the reading time. In four other retrospective tasks, they were not informed about time judgment until they were asked to provide a verbal estimation of four elapsed time intervals (lasting 30 s, 60 s, 90 s, or 120 s). AD participants gave shorter verbal time estimations than older adults and younger participants did, suggesting that time is perceived to pass quickly in these patients. For all participants, the duration of the retrospective tasks was underestimated as compared to the prospective tasks and both estimations were shorter than the real time interval. Prospective time estimation was further correlated with mental time travel, as measured with the Remember/Know paradigm. Mental time travel was even higher correlated with retrospective time estimation. Our findings shed light on the relationship between time perception and the ability to mentally project oneself into time, two skills contributing to human memory functioning. Finally, time perception deficits, as observed in AD patients, can be interpreted in terms of dramatic changes occurring in frontal lobes and hippocampus.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Unlike prospective time perception paradigms, in which participants are aware that they have to estimate forthcoming time, little is known about retrospective time perception in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Our paper addresses this shortcoming by comparing prospective and retrospective time estimation in younger adults, older adults, and AD patients. In four prospective tasks (lasting 30 s, 60 s, 90 s, or 120 s) participants were asked to read a series of numbers and to provide a verbal estimation of the reading time. In four other retrospective tasks, they were not informed about time judgment until they were asked to provide a verbal estimation of four elapsed time intervals (lasting 30 s, 60 s, 90 s, or 120 s). AD participants gave shorter verbal time estimations than older adults and younger participants did, suggesting that time is perceived to pass quickly in these patients. For all participants, the duration of the retrospective tasks was underestimated as compared to the prospective tasks and both estimations were shorter than the real time interval. Prospective time estimation was further correlated with mental time travel, as measured with the Remember/Know paradigm. Mental time travel was even higher correlated with retrospective time estimation. Our findings shed light on the relationship between time perception and the ability to mentally project oneself into time, two skills contributing to human memory functioning. Finally, time perception deficits, as observed in AD patients, can be interpreted in terms of dramatic changes occurring in frontal lobes and hippocampus. |
46. | Boucart, Muriel; Moroni, Christine; Thibaut, Miguel; Szaffarczyk, Sebastien; Greene, Michelle Scene categorization at large visual eccentricities Article de journal Dans: Vision Research, 86 , p. 35–42, 2013, ISSN: 0042-6989. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{boucart_scene_2013,
title = {Scene categorization at large visual eccentricities},
author = {Muriel Boucart and Christine Moroni and Miguel Thibaut and Sebastien Szaffarczyk and Michelle Greene},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698913000977},
doi = {10.1016/j.visres.2013.04.006},
issn = {0042-6989},
year = {2013},
date = {2013-01-01},
urldate = {2015-03-26},
journal = {Vision Research},
volume = {86},
pages = {35--42},
abstract = {Studies of scene perception have shown that the visual system is particularly sensitive to global properties such as the overall layout of a scene. Such global properties cannot be computed locally, but rather require relational analysis over multiple regions. To what extent is observers’ perception of scenes impaired in the far periphery? We examined the perception of global scene properties (Experiment 1) and basic-level categories (Experiment 2) presented in the periphery from 10° to 70°. Pairs of scene photographs were simultaneously presented left and right of fixation for 80 ms on a panoramic screen (5 m diameter) covering the whole visual field while central fixation was controlled. Observers were instructed to press a key corresponding to the spatial location left/right of a pre-defined target property or category. The results show that classification of global scene properties (e.g., naturalness, openness) as well as basic-level categorization (e.g., forests, highways), while better near the center, were accomplished with a performance highly above chance (around 70% correct) in the far periphery even at 70° eccentricity. The perception of some global properties (e.g., naturalness) was more robust in peripheral vision than others (e.g., indoor/outdoor) that required a more local analysis. The results are consistent with studies suggesting that scene gist recognition can be accomplished by the low resolution of peripheral vision.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
Studies of scene perception have shown that the visual system is particularly sensitive to global properties such as the overall layout of a scene. Such global properties cannot be computed locally, but rather require relational analysis over multiple regions. To what extent is observers’ perception of scenes impaired in the far periphery? We examined the perception of global scene properties (Experiment 1) and basic-level categories (Experiment 2) presented in the periphery from 10° to 70°. Pairs of scene photographs were simultaneously presented left and right of fixation for 80 ms on a panoramic screen (5 m diameter) covering the whole visual field while central fixation was controlled. Observers were instructed to press a key corresponding to the spatial location left/right of a pre-defined target property or category. The results show that classification of global scene properties (e.g., naturalness, openness) as well as basic-level categorization (e.g., forests, highways), while better near the center, were accomplished with a performance highly above chance (around 70% correct) in the far periphery even at 70° eccentricity. The perception of some global properties (e.g., naturalness) was more robust in peripheral vision than others (e.g., indoor/outdoor) that required a more local analysis. The results are consistent with studies suggesting that scene gist recognition can be accomplished by the low resolution of peripheral vision. |
2011
|
47. | Baubet, Thierry; Ranque, Brigitte; Taïeb, Olivier; Bérezné, Alice; Bricou, Olivier; Mehallel, Salim; Moroni, Christine; Belin, Catherine; Pagnoux, Christian; Moro, Marie-Rose; Guillevin, Loïc; Mouthon, Luc Mood and anxiety disorders in systemic sclerosis patients. Article de journal Dans: Presse médicale (Paris, France : 1983), 40 (2), p. e111–9, 2011, ISSN: 0755-4982. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{baubet_mood_2011,
title = {Mood and anxiety disorders in systemic sclerosis patients.},
author = {Thierry Baubet and Brigitte Ranque and Olivier Taïeb and Alice Bérezné and Olivier Bricou and Salim Mehallel and Christine Moroni and Catherine Belin and Christian Pagnoux and Marie-Rose Moro and Loïc Guillevin and Luc Mouthon},
url = {http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21055901},
doi = {10.1016/j.lpm.2010.09.019},
issn = {0755-4982},
year = {2011},
date = {2011-01-01},
journal = {Presse médicale (Paris, France : 1983)},
volume = {40},
number = {2},
pages = {e111--9},
abstract = {OBJECTIVE: To assess the prevalence of mood and anxiety disorders in systemic sclerosis (SSc) patients and the association of these disorders with clinical features. METHODS: Between May 2002 and May 2004, 100 SSc patients fulfilling the American Rheumatism Association and/or Leroy & Medsger criteria were recruited: 51 were from a SSc patient association meeting, and 49 were hospitalized in an internal medicine department and recruited consecutively. Mood and anxiety disorders were assessed by use of a structured clinical interview [the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)] performed by a psychiatrist and a self-reporting questionnaire [the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)]. On the same day, psychiatric treatment and clinical features were recorded by a physician. RESULTS: As assessed by the MINI, 19% [95% confidence interval 12-28%] of all SSc patients were currently experiencing a major depressive episode (MDE), 56% [46-65%] had a lifetime history of MDE and 14% [8-22%] had current dysthymia. Current MDE was more prevalent among hospitalized patients than among other patients (28% versus 10%},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
OBJECTIVE: To assess the prevalence of mood and anxiety disorders in systemic sclerosis (SSc) patients and the association of these disorders with clinical features. METHODS: Between May 2002 and May 2004, 100 SSc patients fulfilling the American Rheumatism Association and/or Leroy & Medsger criteria were recruited: 51 were from a SSc patient association meeting, and 49 were hospitalized in an internal medicine department and recruited consecutively. Mood and anxiety disorders were assessed by use of a structured clinical interview [the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)] performed by a psychiatrist and a self-reporting questionnaire [the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)]. On the same day, psychiatric treatment and clinical features were recorded by a physician. RESULTS: As assessed by the MINI, 19% [95% confidence interval 12-28%] of all SSc patients were currently experiencing a major depressive episode (MDE), 56% [46-65%] had a lifetime history of MDE and 14% [8-22%] had current dysthymia. Current MDE was more prevalent among hospitalized patients than among other patients (28% versus 10% |
48. | Bayard, Sophie; Erkes, Jérôme; Moroni, Christine; the des spécialisés en du Roussillon), Collège Psychologues Cliniciens Neuropsychologie Languedoc Roussillon (CPCN Languedoc Victoria Stroop Test: Normative Data in a Sample Group of Older People and the Study of Their Clinical Applications in the Assessment of Inhibition in Alzheimer's Disease Article de journal Dans: Arch Clin Neuropsychol, p. acr053, 2011, ISSN: 0887-6177, 1873-5843. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{bayard_victoria_2011,
title = {Victoria Stroop Test: Normative Data in a Sample Group of Older People and the Study of Their Clinical Applications in the Assessment of Inhibition in Alzheimer's Disease},
author = {Sophie Bayard and Jérôme Erkes and Christine Moroni and Collège Psychologues Cliniciens Neuropsychologie Languedoc Roussillon (CPCN Languedoc the des spécialisés en du Roussillon)},
url = {http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2011/08/26/arclin.acr053},
doi = {10.1093/arclin/acr053},
issn = {0887-6177, 1873-5843},
year = {2011},
date = {2011-01-01},
urldate = {2015-03-26},
journal = {Arch Clin Neuropsychol},
pages = {acr053},
abstract = {The Stroop Color-Word Test-Victoria version (VST) is a measure of executive function commonly used in neuropsychological evaluation. Because of its short administration time, the VST seems particularly appropriate for use in geriatric populations and with those suffering from dementia and who are prone to fatigue during neuropsychological examination. In this study, we examine the influence of demographic characteristics on VST score and present descriptive data for a sample of 244 elderly French speakers (50–94 years of age). Normative data corrected for age and education are provided for clinical use. Furthermore, by comparing the VST performance of patients with the Alzheimer-type dementia to that of 40 matched healthy controls, we provide clinical evidence suggesting that the VST has a clinical utility in the assessment of inhibition in AD.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
The Stroop Color-Word Test-Victoria version (VST) is a measure of executive function commonly used in neuropsychological evaluation. Because of its short administration time, the VST seems particularly appropriate for use in geriatric populations and with those suffering from dementia and who are prone to fatigue during neuropsychological examination. In this study, we examine the influence of demographic characteristics on VST score and present descriptive data for a sample of 244 elderly French speakers (50–94 years of age). Normative data corrected for age and education are provided for clinical use. Furthermore, by comparing the VST performance of patients with the Alzheimer-type dementia to that of 40 matched healthy controls, we provide clinical evidence suggesting that the VST has a clinical utility in the assessment of inhibition in AD. |
2010
|
49. | Rémy, Ludovic; Moroni, Christine Comparaison de trois formes abrégées de l’échelle d’intelligence de Wechsler pour adultes (WAIS-III) In A. De Ribaupierre, P. Ghisletta, Ŧ. Lecerf, J.-L. Roulin (dir), Identité et spécificités de la psychologie différentielle (p.449-454). Article de journal Dans: Presses Universitaires de Rennes, p. 449–454, 2010. BibTeX @article{remy_comparaison_2010,
title = {Comparaison de trois formes abrégées de l’échelle d’intelligence de Wechsler pour adultes (WAIS-III) In A. De Ribaupierre, P. Ghisletta, Ŧ. Lecerf, J.-L. Roulin (dir), Identité et spécificités de la psychologie différentielle (p.449-454).},
author = {Ludovic Rémy and Christine Moroni},
year = {2010},
date = {2010-04-01},
journal = {Presses Universitaires de Rennes},
pages = {449--454},
series = {Presses Universitaires de Rennes.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
|
50. | Boucart, Muriel; Moroni, Christine; Despretz, Pascal; Pasquier, Florence; Fabre-Thorpe, Michele Rapid categorization of faces and objects in a patient with impaired object recognition Article de journal Dans: Neurocase, 16 (2), p. 157–168, 2010, ISSN: 1355-4794. Résumé | Liens | BibTeX @article{boucart_rapid_2010,
title = {Rapid categorization of faces and objects in a patient with impaired object recognition},
author = {Muriel Boucart and Christine Moroni and Pascal Despretz and Florence Pasquier and Michele Fabre-Thorpe},
url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554790903339637},
doi = {10.1080/13554790903339637},
issn = {1355-4794},
year = {2010},
date = {2010-03-01},
urldate = {2015-03-26},
journal = {Neurocase},
volume = {16},
number = {2},
pages = {157--168},
abstract = {We tested rapid-categorization in a patient who was impaired in face and object recognition. Photographs of natural scenes were displayed for 100 ms. Participants had to press a key when they saw an animal among various objects as distractors or human faces among animal faces as distractors. Though the patient was impaired at figure/ground segregation, recognized very few objects and faces, she categorized animals and faces with a performance ranging between 70 and 86% correct. Displaying pictures in isolation did not improve performance. The results suggest that rapid categorization can be accomplished on the basis of coarse information without overt recognition.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}
We tested rapid-categorization in a patient who was impaired in face and object recognition. Photographs of natural scenes were displayed for 100 ms. Participants had to press a key when they saw an animal among various objects as distractors or human faces among animal faces as distractors. Though the patient was impaired at figure/ground segregation, recognized very few objects and faces, she categorized animals and faces with a performance ranging between 70 and 86% correct. Displaying pictures in isolation did not improve performance. The results suggest that rapid categorization can be accomplished on the basis of coarse information without overt recognition. |